I taught this mental math lesson to a class of fourth graders. I chose an addition problem—99 + 17—for the students to solve mentally, purposely selecting a problem that would be accessible to the students. Also, I knew that the problem could be solved in different ways. I planned to elicit from the students strategies for figuring out the sum of 116 and then to show video clips of other students solving the same problem. After watching each video clip, we’d analyze how the student reasoned and then check back to see if anyone in our class used the same strategy.
I present this lesson in a video that’s similar to the one I made for my previous post. It’s basically a Power Point presentation that I’ve narrated to describe how the lesson actually unfolded with fourth graders. The presentation runs just about 8 minutes. I’m interested in your feedback about if the video is useful. Also, if you watched the presentation from my previous post where I reproduced how I recorded on the board instead of taking photos of what I did, I’m interested if one format is more useful or appealing than the other.
Here’s how the lesson went.
A Repeat Experience
I led a similar lesson for the problem 15 x 12. Again, I selected three videos or interviews from the MRI site to show the class how Monica, Malcolm, and Alberto figured out mentally that the answer was 180. I chose these particular clips because, as for 99 + 17, the students all figured out the correct answer. Also, while they all applied the distributive property, they each took a different numerical approach to the problem.
The lesson didn’t work this time! That’s because I changed the sequence from what I had done with 99 + 17. For 15 x 12, I showed the video clips to the students in the class first, before asking them to engage with the problem, figure out the answer for themselves, and report their strategies. I didn’t initially record solution strategies on the board as I had done for 99 + 17. Showing the clips for 15 x 12 to the students before they had a chance to think about the problem for themselves was not a good instructional decision. Without the initial investment of their own thinking, the students weren’t able to or interested in engaging with the problem on the video clips. They didn’t have a context for listening to the students interviewed in the video clips.
On reflection, this was a “what-was-I-thinking” experience, a pedagogical error that I won’t repeat. It’s important for students to buy in first.
Questions Teachers Have Asked
I’ve shared this Power Point presentation in sessions with teachers. Below are a few of the questions they asked and the responses I gave. If you have any other questions, post them and I’ll respond.
Q.What if only one or two students have an idea for how to compute 99 + 17?
A. In this situation, I typically do one of two things. Sometimes I offer a strategy and record it as I explain (labeling it Ms. Burns as I label theirs with their names). Another is to acknowledge that the ideas on the board were the only ones they had so far, and ask them to turn and talk with a partner to see if they can come up with another strategy.
Q. Why do you write students’ names on the board when they report?
A. Two reasons. One is it makes it easier to refer back to particular strategies. Another is that it honors the student who reported, which I think is important.
Q. I noticed that in some of the video clips of interviews on the MRI site, students answer incorrectly. Do you show these video clips as well and ask students to figure out the error?
A. While I think the video clips of students making errors can help us as teachers, I do not choose to show them to students. I prefer to select examples from which students can learn appropriate strategies.
Q. What do you mean by “strategies appropriate to the numbers at hand”?
A. One of the MRI video clips for 99 + 17 that I chose not to show to the fourth graders shows Zakari solving the problem by counting up by ones from 99. While counting on is an acceptable strategy for younger students, I don’t think it’s acceptable for fifth graders. Watch Mallika Scott interviewing Zakari.
Similarly, a video clip of Ana shows her using her finger on the desktop as if she were solving the problem with paper and pencil. Watch Leo Kostelnik interviewing Ana.
While neither of these video clips by themselves indicate that Zakari or Ana necessarily lacks number sense, it’s important to check that students are able to reason numerically by applying place value and properties of numbers. When answering other questions, Zakari demonstrated that he had other strategies to rely on. Ana, however, didn’t and always resorted to algorithms, even for the problem 1000 – 998. She wore her finger smooth on the desktop.
This is a solid reminder that the same, exact delivery can bring utterly different results depending on how the stage before that delivery is set. Thanks! I struggle to understand (and when I do, I struggle to explain) why some students just don’t engage in explanations — and that it’s not a character flaw on their part. It’s what they’ve learned from experience.
This is so incredibly helpful to me! I am a fourth-grade teacher and I can’t wait to try this with my class. Thank you for the video clips.
It is interesting that for 15 x 12 in MRI “they all applied the distributive property.”
I would have guessed that, given multiple solutions, at least one of them would use factoring:
15×12 = 3x5x2x6 = (3×6)x(2×5) = 18×10 = 180.
MQ
https://mathwater.wordpress.com/
We interviewed many students when developing the MRI tool, and factoring never came up. I think this might have been because we ask students to solve the problem in their heads, without the benefit of paper & pencil to keep track of factors. Also, the instructional emphasis for the students in the video clips was on splitting numbers and using the distributive property. It would be interesting to present these students with your method and see if they could figure out why it also makes sense.
In my experience, students who have been exposed to the method of matching up 2s and 5s can solve such problems “in their heads.” But I have also observed an underemphasis on factoring as a tool for elementary school teachers.
(I have presented the following sort of question, which you should feel free to use or adapt: “Marilyn solved the problem 75×36 in her head by multiplying 100×27. What was Marilyn thinking when she rewrote the expression as 100×27? Why do you think she chose those numbers? Use Marilyn’s method to calculate mentally: 18×35. How might you rewrite this expression?”)
An interesting follow-up – though this one is probably best done with paper & pencil! – is to consider with students which numbers can be multiplied with the help of factoring. At some point, the conversation can be moved towards why multiplying two prime numbers isn’t especially helped by factorization.
My favorite example: 43×37.
Factorization is of no real help here, but the distributive law can be used quite effectively! It is difficult to scaffold so that this insight arises organically, but:
43×37 = (40+3)(40-3) = 40^2 – 3^2 = 1600 – 9 = 1591.
MQ
https://mathwater.wordpress.com/
The videos are perfect! I use number talks similar to this in my classroom. I love that you had the kiddos watch the videos and look for a matching strategy. This definitely is higher order thinking. Thank you for sharing; I LOVE your blog!!
Brooke
Tales from a Fourth Grade MathNut